Exercício aeróbio agudo e pensamento criativo: comparação do estado de hipofrontalidade transitória e o modelo de força das predições teóricas de autocontrole

Autores

DOI:

https://doi.org/10.47197/retos.v58.103685

Palavras-chave:

Cognition, Convergent, Creativity, Divergent, Self-depletion, Acute Exercise

Resumo

A criatividade é uma atividade cognitiva de alto nível que pode ser afetada pelo exercício aeróbico agudo. Este estudo tem como objetivo determinar como intensidades agudas de exercício aeróbio podem influenciar aspectos do pensamento criativo divergente e convergente. Nesse sentido, para dar uma visão dos resultados, foram levadas em consideração as previsões da tese da hipofrontalidade transitória e do modelo de força do autocontrole. Noventa e três estudantes universitários saudáveis ​​(idade = 21,18 ± 1,73 anos) foram aleatoriamente designados para intensidade do limiar anaeróbico (%85 FCmáx, n = 31), baixa intensidade (%60 FCmáx, n = 31) e controle (n = 31). grupos = 31). Os participantes realizaram tarefas de pensamento criativo divergentes (Tarefa de Usos Alternativos) e convergentes (Testes Progressivos Avançados de Raven) antes, durante e após a cessação de duas intensidades de limiar anaeróbico, baixa intensidade e condições de controle. Para o pensamento criativo divergente, o desempenho da flexibilidade e da fluência durante e após a cessação do exercício nos grupos de intensidade do limiar anaeróbico e de baixa intensidade melhorou em comparação com o grupo de controle. Esses resultados foram consistentes com a tese da hipofrontalidade transitória e inconsistentes com as previsões do modelo de força de autocontrole. Em contraste, o desempenho do pensamento criativo convergente foi relativamente estável durante e após a cessação do exercício nos grupos de intensidade do limiar anaeróbico e de baixa intensidade, o que foi inconsistente com as previsões tanto da tese da hipofrontalidade transitória como do modelo de força de autocontrole. Os resultados sugerem que as previsões teóricas da tese da hipofrontalidade transitória são mais adequadas à literatura do que o modelo de força de autocontrole para considerar a relação entre exercício e processos criativos, e estudos adicionais sobre o tema seriam de interesse.

Palavras-chave: Cognição, Convergente, Criatividade, Divergente, Autoexaustão, Atividade Física)

Referências

Abraham, A. (2016). Gender and creativity: an overview of psychological and neuroscientific literature. Brain Imaging and Behavior, 2, 609-618. doi:10.1007/s11682-015-9410-8

Aga, K., Inamura, M., Chen, C., Hagiwara, K., Yamashita, R., Hirotsu, M., Seki, T., Takao, A., Fujii, Y., Matsubara, T., & Nakagawa, S. (2021). The effect of acute aerobic exercise on divergent and convergent thinking and its influence by mood. Brain Sciences, 11(5), 546. https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci11050546

Akbari Chermahini, S., & Hommel, B. (2010). The (b) link between creativity and dopamine: spontaneous eye blink rates predict and dissociate divergent and convergent thinking. Cognition, 115(3), 458-465. https://doi.org/ https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2010.03.007

American College of Sports Medicine. (2018). ACSM’s Exercise Testing and Prescription. Philadelphia, PA: Lippincott williams & wilkins.

Audiffren, M., & Andre, N. (2015). The strength model of self-control revisited: Linking acute and chronic effects of exercise on executive functions. Journal of Sport and Health Science, 4, 30-46. https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jshs.2014.09.002

Audiffren, M., Tomporowski, P. D., & Zagrondik, J. (2008). Acute aerobic exercise and information processing: Energizing motor processes during a choice reaction time tasks. Acta Psychologica, 129, 410-419. https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2008.09.006

Baumeister, R., Bratslavsky, E., Muraven, M., & Tice, D. (1998). Ego depletion: is the active self a limited resource? Journal of personality and social psychology, 74(5), 1252–1265. https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.74.5.1252

Baumeister, R., & Vohs, K. (2007). Self-regulation, ego depletion, and motivation. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 1, 1-14. https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1751-9004.2007.00001.x

Benton, D. (2011). Dehydration influences mood and cognition: a plausible hypothesis? Nutrients, 3, 555 – 573. https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.3390/nu3050555

Blanchette, D. M., Ramocki, S. P., O’del, J. N., & Casey, M. S. (2005). Aerobic exercise and creative potential: Immediate and residual effects. Creativity Research Journal, 17(2-3), 257-264. https://doi.org/https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1080/10400419.2005.9651483

Cantelon, J. A., & Giles, G. E. (2021). A Review of Cognitive Changes During Acute Aerobic Exercise. Frontiers in Psychology, 12, 1-29. https://doi.org/doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.653158

Chang, Y., Labban, J., Gapin, J., & Etnier, J. (2012). The effects of acute exercise on cognitive performance: a meta-analysis. Brain Research, 1453, 87-101. https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2012.02.068

Chen, C. (2024). Exploring the impact of acute physical activity on creative thinking: a comprehensive narrative review with a focus on activity type and intensity. Discover Psychology, 4(3), 1-19. doi:https://doi.org/10.1007/s44202-024-00114-9

Clapham, M. (2003). The development of innovative ideas through creativity training. In The international handbook on innovation (pp. 366–376). NY: Elsevier Science. . https://doi.org/https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1016/b978-008044198-6/50025-5

Clarkson, J., Hirt, E., Jia, L., & Alexander, M. (2010). When perception is more than reality: The effect of perceived versus actual resource depletion on self-regulatory behavior. Journal of personality and social psychology, 98, 29-46. https://doi.org/ https://doi.org/10.1037/a0017539

Colzato, L. S., Szapora, A., Pannekoek, J. N., & Hommel, B. (2013). The impact of physical exercise on convergent and divergent thinking. Frontiers in human neuroscience, 7, 1-6. https://doi.org/https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2013.00824

Corgnet, B., Espín, A. M., & Hernán-González, R. (2016). Creativity and cognitive skills among millennials: thinking too much and creating too little. Frontiers in psychology, 7, 1-9. https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01626

Cortes, R. A., Weinberger, A. B., Daker, R. J., & Green, A. E. (2019). Re-examining prominent measures of divergent and convergent creativity Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 27, 90–93. https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cobeha.2018.09.017

Davranche, K., Brisswalter, J., & Radel, R. (2015). Where are the limits of the effects of exercise intensity on cognitive control? . Journal of Sport and Health Science, 4, 56-63. https://doi.org/http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jshs.2014.08.004

de Rooij, A., & Vromans, R. (2020). The (dis) pleasures of creativity: Spontaneous eye blink rate during divergent and convergent thinking depends on individual differences in positive and negative affect The Journal of Creative Behavior, 54(2), 436–452. https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1002/jocb.379

Del Giorno, J. M., Hall, E. E., O'Leary, K. C., Bixby, W. R., & Miller, P. C. (2010). Cognitive function during acute exercise: A test of the Transient Hypofrontality Theory. Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology, 32, 312-323. https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1123/jsep.32.3.312

Diamond, A. (2000). Close interrelation of motor development and cognitive development and of the cerebellum and prefrontal cortex. Child Development, 71, 44-56. doi:https://www.jstor.org/stable/1132216

Dietrich, A. (2003). Functionl neuroanatomy of altered states of consciousness: the transient hypofrontality hypothesis. Conscious and Cognition, 12, 231-256. https://doi.org/https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1016/S1053-8100(02)00046-6

Dietrich, A. (2004). The cognitive neuroscience of creativity. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 11(6), 1011–1026. https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.3758/bf03196731

Ellis, H. (1894/1934). Man and Woman: A Study of Human Sexual Characters. London: Heinemann.

Fink, A., Reim, T., Benedek, M., & Grabner, R. (2019). The effects of a verbal and a figural creativity training on different facets of creative potential. The Journal of Creative Behavior, 53, 1-10. https://doi.org/ https://doi.org/10.1002/jocb.402

Fink, A., Reim, T., Benedek, M., & Grabner, R. H. (2020). The Effects of a Verbal and a Figural Creativity Training on Different Facets of Creative Potential. The Journal of Creative Behavior, 54(3), 676–685. https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1002/jocb.402

Foster, P., Williamson, J., & Harrison, D. (2005). The Ruff Figural Fluency Test: Heightened right frontal lobe delta activity as a function of performance. . Archives of Clinical Neuropsychological, 20, 427-434. https://doi.org/http://doi:10.1016/j.acn.2004.09.010

Frith, E., Miller, S. E., & Loprinzi, P. D. (2020). Effects of Verbal Priming With Acute Exercise on Convergent Creativity. Psychological Reports, 125, 375 - 397. https://doi.org/doi: 10.1177/0033294120981925.

Globus, M., Melamed, E., & Karen, E. (1983). Effect of exercise on cerebrel circulation. Journal of Cerebrel Blood Flow and Metabolism, 3, 287-290. https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1038/jcbfm.1983.43

Guilford, J. P. (1967). The nature of human intelligence. New York: McGraw-Hill. https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.3102/00028312005002249

Hagger, M., Wood , C., Stiff , C., & Chatzisarantis, N. (2010). Ego depletion and the strength model of self-control: a meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 136, 495-525. https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1037/a0019486

He, W., & Wong, W. (2011). Gender differences in creative thinking revisited: Findings from analysis of variability. Personality and Individual Differences, 51(7), 807-811. https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2011.06.027

He, W., & Wong, W. (2021). Gender Differences in the Distribution of Creativity Scores: Domain-Specific Patterns in Divergent Thinking and Creative Problem Solving. Frontiers in Psychology, 12, 1-14. https://doi.org/doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.626911

Heilman, K. M., Nadeau, S. E., & Beversdorf, D. O. (2003). Creative innovation: possible brain mechanisms. Neurocase 9(5), 369-379. https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1076/neur.9.5.369.16553

Jaarsveld, S., Lachmann, T., & van Leeuwen, C. (2012). Creative reasoning across developmental levels: Convergence and divergence in problem creation. Intelligence, 40(2), 172–188. https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2012.01.002

Karwowski, M., Jankowska, D., Gralewski, J., Gajda, A., Wiśniewska, E., & Lebuda, I. (2016). Greater male variability in creativity: a latent variables approach. Thinking Skills and Creativity, 22, 159-166. doi:10.1016/j.tsc.2016.10.005

Kemppainen, J., Aalto, S., Fujimoto, T., Kalliokski, K. K., Langsjo, J., Oikonen, V., Rinne, J., Nuutila, P., & Knuuti, J. (2005). High intensity exercise decreases global brain glucose uptake in humans. The Journal of Physiology 568, 323-332. https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1113/jphysiol.2005.091355

Kounios, J., & Beeman, M. (2014). The cognitive neuroscience of insight.

. Annual review of psychology, 65 71-93. https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-010213-115154

Kujach, S., Byun, K., Hyodo, K., Suwabe, K., Fukuie, T., Laskowski, R., Dan, I., & Soya, H. (2018). A transferable high-intensity intermittent exercise improves executive performance in association with dorsolateral prefrontal activation in young adults. Neuroimage, 169, 117–125. https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.12.003

Lambourne, K., Audiffren, M., & Tomporowski , P. D. (2010). Effects of acute exercise on sensory and executive processing tasks. Medicine & Science in Sport & Exercise, 42(7), 1396-1402. https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1249/MSS.0b013e3181cbee11

Lambourne, K., & Tomporowski, P. (2010). The effect of exercise-induced arousal on cognitive task performance: A meta-regression analysis. Brain Research, 1341, 12-24. https://doi.org/https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2010.03.091

Matsumoto, K., Chen, C., Hagiwara, K., Shimizu, N., Hirotsu, M., Oda, Y., Lei, H., Takao, A., Fujii, Y., Higuchi, F., & Nakagawa, S. (2022). The Effect of Brief Stair-Climbing on Divergent and Convergent Thinking. Frontiers in behavioral neuroscience, 15, 1-7. https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.3389/fnbeh.2021.834097

Miller, E. K., & Cohen, J. D. (2001). An integrative theory of prefrontal cortex function. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 24(1), 167-202. https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.neuro.24.1.167

Oppezzo, M., & Schwartz, D. L. (2014). Give your ideas some legs: The positive effect of walking on creative thinking. : Learning, Memory, and Cognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 40(4), 1142–1152. https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1037/a0036577

Pinto, S. S., Brasil, R. M., Alberton, C. L., Ferreira, H. A., Bagatini, N. C., Calatayud, J., & Colado, J. C. (2016). Noninvasive determination of anaerobic threshold based on the heart rate deflection point in water cycling. The Journal of Strength & Conditioning Research, 30(2), 518-524. https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1519/JSC.0000000000001099

Pires, F., Dos Anjos, C., Covolan, R., Fontes, E., Noakes, T., Gibson, A., Magalhaes, F. H., & Ugrinowitsch, C. (2018). Caffeine and placebo improved maximal exercise performance despite unchanged motor cortex activation and greater prefrontal cortex deoxygenation. Frontiers in Physiology, 9, 1-11. https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.3389/fphys.2018.01144

Polevoy, G. (2024). The effect of aerobic running on children's thinking and endurance. Retos, 54, 303-311. doi:https://doi.org/10.47197/retos.v54.103477

Pontifex, M. B., McGowan, A. L., Chandler, M. C., Gwizdala, K. L., Parks, A. C., Fenn, K., & Kamijo, K. (2019). A primer on investigating the after effects of acute bouts of physical activity on cognition. Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 40, 1–22. https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychsport.2018.08.015

Salmon, P. (2001). Effects of physical exercise on anxiety, depression, and sensitivity to stress: A unifying theory. Clinical Psychology Review, 21, 33-61. https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1016/S0272-7358(99)00032-X

Schmeichel, B., & Baumeister, R. (2010). Effortful attention control. In B. Bruya (Ed.), Effortless Attention: A New Perspective in the Cognitive Science of Attention and Action (pp. 29–49). Cambridge,MA: MIT Press. https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.7551/mitpress/9780262013840.003.0002

Sniehotta , F., Scholz U, U., & Schwarzer , R. (2005). Bridging the intention behaviour gap: planning, self-efficacy, and action control in the adoption and maintenance of physical exercise. Psychology & Health, 20, 143-160. https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1080/08870440512331317670

Tanaka, H., Monahan, K. D., & Seals, D. R. (2001). Age-predicted maximal heart rate revisited. Journal of the American College of Cardiology 37(1), 153-156. https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1016/S0735-1097(00)01054-8

Tomporowski, P. D. (2003). Effects of acute bout of exercise on cognition. Acta Psychologica, 112, 297-324. https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1016/s0001-6918(02)00134-8

Tyler , J., & Burns, K. (2008). After Depletion: The Replenishment of the Self's Regulatory Resources. Self and Identity, 7(3), 305-321. https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1080/15298860701799997

Zhao, Y., Qin, C., Shu, D., & Liu, D. (2022). Effects of short-term aerobic exercise on creativity. Thinking Skills and Creativity, 44. https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tsc.2022.101033

Downloads

Publicado

2024-09-01

Como Citar

Maleki, M., Ahmadpour, A. ., Moraru, A. ., & Memmert, D. . (2024). Exercício aeróbio agudo e pensamento criativo: comparação do estado de hipofrontalidade transitória e o modelo de força das predições teóricas de autocontrole. Retos, 58, 880–890. https://doi.org/10.47197/retos.v58.103685

Edição

Secção

Artigos de caráter científico: trabalhos de pesquisas básicas e/ou aplicadas.

Artigos mais lidos do(s) mesmo(s) autor(es)