ALICIA LÓPEZ-ÁLVAREZ(1), LUCA PIOVANO(2), FRANCISCO LUQUE(2) y CARLOS DE ALDAMA(1)
(1) Complutense University of Madrid (Spain)
(2) Polytechnic University of Madrid (Spain)
DOI: 10.13042/Bordon.2024.100116
Fecha de recepción: 31/05/2023 • Fecha de aceptación: 10/04/2024
Autora de contacto / Corresponding autor: Alicia López Álvarez. E-mail: aliclo14@ucm.es
Cómo citar este artículo: López-Álvarez, A., Piovano, L. Luque, F. y de Aldama, C. de (2024). Enhancing safe pedestrian behavior through virtual reality: an empirical study. Bordón, Revista de Pedagogía, 76(3), 99-123. https://doi.org/10.13042/Bordon.2024.100116
INTRODUCTION. Over the last few years, the number of pedestrian fatalities on urban roads has increased, largely due to infractions associated with their behaviors (e.g., crossing when traffic lights are red). It is argued that these behaviors reflect a lack of risk perception. Road safety programs have tried to raise awareness through various methods, using quite often emotionally powerful experiences (e.g. testimonies of people who have experienced an accident themselves). Recently, Virtual Reality (VR) has been deployed with the aim of increasing the efficacy of these safety programs. Previous studies have demonstrated the potential of VR to improve pedestrian safety, especially when it is accompanied bydebriefing and critical reflection. METHOD. A total of 43 participants (M = 24.5 years old; SD = 5.14; 65.12% female) were involved in an experimental study with a 2x2 factorial design and pre-post measures. They were randomly assigned to one of four groups (Experience a VR accident /Experience VR without an accident; having a debriefing after the VR experience/not having a debriefing after the VR experience). Pre-post measures were of two kinds, (a) self-report measures and (b)VR behavioral measures. Multivariate Analysis of Variance (MANOVA) and general linear mixed models (GLMM) were used to analyze the data. RESULTS AND DISCUSSION. The main results revealed that (a) participants reported a general reduction in the number of rules violations, regardless of condition, and (b) there was a significant reduction in the number of violations committed in VR (i.e., crossing when the traffic light is red) in the condition where participants had previously experienced an accident. These results support the potential of using VR environments to improve pedestrian safety-related behavior. Implications for future research are delineated.
Keywords: Virtual reality (VR), Debriefing techniques, Safety programs, Pedestrian behavior, Accident risk, Safety.
According to World Health Organization (2018World Health Organization (2018). Global status report on road safety. ), pedestrians, cyclists and motorcyclists represented more than half of global road traffic deaths (28% motorized 2-3 wheelers, 23% pedestrians and 3% cyclists). Consequently, the United Nations General Assembly (2020Secretary-General, U. N. (2020). Improving global road safety: note/by the Secretary-General.) proclaimed the Decade of Action for Road Safety 2021-2030 with the goal of improving global road safety and preventing at least 50% of road traffic deaths and injuries by 2030. In the case of Spain, pedestrians are also among the most vulnerable groups of users on roads, accounting for 11% of the total number (126 pedestrians) of deaths in 2022 (National Road Safety Observatory, 2023National Road Safety Observatory (2023). Siniestralidad mortal en vías interurbanas 2022. Dirección General de Tráfico de Madrid, España.). Specifically, the percentage of pedestrians killed in urban areas in Spain was 44%, compared to the European Union’s average of 37%. In addition, the number of pedestrian fatalities over the year of 2022 increased 7%.
Urban roads accounted for the highest proportion of accidents involving pedestrians (93% of these accidents, 61% of pedestrian fatalities, and 89% of pedestrian injuries in hospitals), according to National Road Safety Observatory (2021National Road Safety Observatory (2021). Las primeras cifras de la siniestralidad de los peatones. Dirección General de Tráfico de Madrid, España.). The most prominent unsafe actions by pedestrians in urban roads were walking on the roadway or roadside (21%), crossing the road outside an intersection (17%), and crossing an intersection (14%). Furthermore, 53% of pedestrians involved in an accident committed a violation, such as being or walking on the road in an unregulated way (19%), not using zebra crossings to cross (8%), and other offenses (6%). This high number of transgressions and the relatively low frequency of pedestrian accidents may reduce the risk perception associated with these behaviors (Granié et al., 2013Granié, M. A., Pannetier, M. & Gueho, L. (2013). Developing a self-reporting method to measure pedestrian behaviors at all ages. Accident Analysis & Prevention, 50, 830-839. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.aap.2012.07.009). Consequently, pedestrians with scarce traffic safety consciousness usually developed hazardous traffic behaviors such as running a red light and diverted crossing (Hou et al., 2022Hou, M., Chen, S. & Cheng, J. (2022). The effect of risk perception and other psychological factors on mobile phone use while crossing the street among pedestrians. Accident Analysis & Prevention, 170. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.aap.2022.106643).
Road safety programs have traditionally increased the risk perception of pedestrians to decrease fatalities (Assailly, 2017Assailly, J. P. (2017). Road safety education: What works? Patient education and counseling, 100, S24-S29. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pec.2015.10.017; Twisk et al., 2014Twisk, D. A., Vlakveld, W. P., Commandeur, J. J., Shope, J. T. & Kok, G. (2014). Five road safety education programmes for young adolescent pedestrians and cyclists: A multi-programme evaluation in a field setting. Accident Analysis & Prevention, 66, 55-61. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.aap.2014.01.002). For example, raising awareness through generating emotional impact on the individual can be accomplished by sharing the testimony of people who have suffered serious accidents or using explicit media advertising campaigns (Delgado, 2021Delgado, A. (2021). Campañas: ¿la clave? Repetir. Revista DGT. https://revista.dgt.es/es/reportajes/2021/09SEPTIEMBRE/0923-Campanas-publicidad.shtml).
Advances in technology, including Virtual Reality (VR) or Augmented Reality (AR), have helped develop more effective educational intervention programs (Ferguson et al., 2020Ferguson, C., Van den Broek, E. L. & Van Oostendorp, H. (2020). On the role of interaction mode and story structure in virtual reality serious games. Computers & Education, 143, 103671. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.compedu.2019.103671; Seo et al., 2021Seo, H. J., Park, G. M., Son, M. & Hong, A. J. (2021). Establishment of virtual-reality-based safety education and training system for safety engagement. Education Sciences, 11(12), 786. https://doi.org/10.3390/educsci11120786; Purcell & Romjin, 2020Purcell, C. & Romijn, A. (2020). Teaching children road safety using a simulated environment. Journal of Education and Educational Development, 7(1), 44-54. https://doi.org/10.22555/joeed.v7i1.2948; Vankov & Jankovsky, 2021Vankov, D. & Jankovszky, D. (2021). Effects of using headset-delivered virtual reality in road safety research: A systematic review of empirical studies. Virtual Reality & Intelligent Hardware, 3(5), 351-368. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.vrih.2021.05.005; Marrero Galván, & Hernández Padrón, 2022Marrero Galván, J. J. & Hernández Padrón, M. (2022). La trascendencia de la realidad virtual en la educación STEM: una revisión sistemática desde el punto de vista de la experimentación en el aula. Bordón. Revista de Pedagogía, 74(4), 45-63. https://doi.org/10.13042/Bordon.2022.94179; Palacios Ortega et al., 2022Palacios Ortega, A., Pascual López, V. & Moreno Mediavilla, D. (2022). El papel de las nuevas tecnologías en la educación STEM. Bordón. Revista de Pedagogía, 74(4), 11-21. https://doi.org/10.13042/Bordon.2022.96550; Baeza González et al., 2023Baeza González, A., Usart Rodríguez, M. & Marqués Molías, L. (2023). Un análisis de las simulaciones virtuales desde la óptica del modelo TPACK. Bordón. Revista de Pedagogía, 75(4), 109-133. https://doi.org/10.13042/Bordon.2023.97585) and provide new opportunities for human factors research in areas that are dangerous or difficult to study in the real world (Deb et al., 2017aDeb, S., Carruth, D. W., Sween, R., Strawderman, L. & Garrison, T. M. (2017a). Efficacy of virtual reality in pedestrian safety research. Applied Ergonomics, 65, 449-460. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.apergo.2017.03.007). Importantly, an overall increase in pedestrian awareness after exposure to simulated environments has been demonstrated to be transferable to real road traffic contexts (Cook et al., 2013Cook, D. A., Stanley, J. H., Brydges, R., Zendejas B, Szostek, J. H., Wang, T., Erwin, P. J. & Hatala, R. (2013). Comparative effectiveness of instructional design features in simulation-based education: Systematic review and meta-analysis. Medical Teacher, 35(1), e867-e898. https://doi.org/10.3109/0142159X.2012.714886; Çakiroğlu & Gökoğlu, 2019Çakiroğlu, Ü., & Gökoğlu, S. (2019). Development of fire safety behavioral skills via virtual reality. Computers & Education, 133, 56-68. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.compedu.2019.01.014; Saadati et al., 2022Saadati, M., Razzaghi, A., Rezapour, R. & Pourebrahim, K. (2022). Interventions for safety promotion of pedestrians; A scoping review. Journal of Transport & Health, 24. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jth.2021.101277), which can be due to (1) the development of many skills without fear of negative consequences (e.g., learning to pilot an airplane, see Aguilar-Reyes et al., 2022Aguilar-Reyes, C., Wozniak, D., Ham, A. & Zahabi, M. (2022). An adaptive virtual reality-based training system for pilots. In Proceedings of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society Annual Meeting (vol. 66, n.º 1, pp. 1962-1966)., CA: SAGE Publications. https://doi.org/10.1177/1071181322661063); and/or (2) self-reflection on one‘s own experience in a way that allows one to become aware of what has been learned (Gardner, 2013Gardner, R. (2013). Introduction to debriefing. In Seminars in perinatology, vol. 37, n.o 3, 166-174. WB Saunders. https://doi.org/10.1053/j.semperi.2013.02.008). In line with the latter, some authors (Levett-Jones & Lapkin, 2014Levett-Jones, T. & Lapkin, S. (2014). A systematic review of the effectiveness of simulation debriefing in health professional education. Nurse Education Today, 34(6), e58-e63. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.nedt.2013.09.020; Feng et al., 2021Feng, Q., Luo, H., Li, W., Chen, Y.,& Zhang, J. (2021). The moderating effect of debriefing on learning outcomes of IVR-based instruction: an experimental research. Applied Sciences, 11(21), 10426. https://doi.org/10.3390/app112110426) recommend following the simulated intervention with the debriefing technique, a method that develops critical conversations and engages learners to discover the thinking behind the actions performed during the simulation (Lee et al., 2020Lee, J., Lee, H., Kim, S., Choi, M., Ko, I. S., Bae, J. & Kim, S. H. (2020). Debriefing methods and learning outcomes in simulation nursing education: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Nurse Education Today, 87. https://doi.org./10.1016/j.nedt.2020.104345). Facilitating debriefing immediately after the intervention has been encouraged to make the experience as fresh as possible (Gardner, 2013Gardner, R. (2013). Introduction to debriefing. In Seminars in perinatology, vol. 37, n.o 3, 166-174. WB Saunders. https://doi.org/10.1053/j.semperi.2013.02.008).
It is worth noting that human factors are the primary cause of road accidents, constituting up to 90% of incidents (Gicquel et al., 2017Gicquel, L., Ordonneau, P., Blot, E., Toillon, C., Ingrand, P. & Romo, L. (2017). Description of various factors contributing to traffic accidents in youth and measures proposed to alleviate recurrence. Frontiers in psychiatry, 8, 94. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2017.00094). Indeed, these factors significantly influence pedestrian behavior, shaping risk-taking tendencies and exposure levels (Deb et al., 2017aDeb, S., Carruth, D. W., Sween, R., Strawderman, L. & Garrison, T. M. (2017a). Efficacy of virtual reality in pedestrian safety research. Applied Ergonomics, 65, 449-460. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.apergo.2017.03.007; Wang et al., 2010Wang, T., Wu, J., Zheng, P. & McDonald, M. (2010). Study of pedestrians’ gap acceptance behavior when they jaywalk outside crossing facilities. In 13th International IEEE Conference on Intelligent Transportation Systems (pp. 1295-1300). IEEE. https://doi.org/ 10.1109/ITSC.2010.5625157.). Valuable insights by eliciting self-reports on walking and crossing practices, thus, enable the exploration of crucial accident-related behaviors (e.g., traffic violations) and their underlying psychological mechanisms (Zhou & Horrey, 2010Zhou, R. & Horrey, W. J. (2010). Predicting adolescent pedestrians’ behavioral intentions to follow the masses in risky crossing situations. Transportation research part F: traffic psychology and behavior, 13(3), 153-163. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.trf.2009.12.001; Granié et al., 2013Granié, M. A., Pannetier, M. & Gueho, L. (2013). Developing a self-reporting method to measure pedestrian behaviors at all ages. Accident Analysis & Prevention, 50, 830-839. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.aap.2012.07.009; O‘Hern et al., 2019O’Hern, S., Stephens, A. N., Young, K. & Koppel, S., (2019). Personality traits as predictors of cyclist behavior. In: Paper Presented at the International Cycling Safety Conference – ICSC2019, 18-20 November 2019 (extended Version). Brisbane, Australia. QUT, Brisbane. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.aap.2020.105704).
Traditionally, the behavior of street users can be classified according to deviations from common practices (Deb et al., 2017bDeb, S., Strawderman, L., DuBien, J., Smith, B., Carruth, D. W. & Garrison, T. M. (2017b). Evaluating pedestrian behavior at crosswalks: Validation of a pedestrian behavior questionnaire for the US population. Accident Analysis & Prevention, 106, 191-201. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.aap.2017.05.020; Reason et al., 1990Reason, J., Manstead, A., Stradling, S., Baxter, J. & Campbell, K. (1990). Errors and violations on the roads: a real distinction? Ergonomics, 33(10-11), 1315-1332. https://doi.org/10.1080/00140139008925335). If the deviation is unintentional, the action shall be classified as an error. Instead, if the deviation is intentional, the action is classified as a violation. Because intentional deviations from social norms are usually not intended to cause injury or harm, it is therefore reasonable to conclude that attitudes towards traffic rule violations are related to the distracted behavior of pedestrians (Hou et al., 2022Hou, M., Chen, S. & Cheng, J. (2022). The effect of risk perception and other psychological factors on mobile phone use while crossing the street among pedestrians. Accident Analysis & Prevention, 170. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.aap.2022.106643). Unintentional deficiencies in the decision-making involved in choosing a goal and/or the resources for reaching it can be of two types (Reason et al., 1990Reason, J., Manstead, A., Stradling, S., Baxter, J. & Campbell, K. (1990). Errors and violations on the roads: a real distinction? Ergonomics, 33(10-11), 1315-1332. https://doi.org/10.1080/00140139008925335). On the one hand, knowledge errors occur when finding the right solution to a new problem requires a trial-and-error procedure, and on the other hand, rule-related errors occur when a pre-established performance rule is inappropriately implemented to the situation, whether the individual applies the correct rule incorrectly or not at all or applies the wrong rule.
Finally, positive behaviors in pedestrians are those that are defined as behaviors that seek to foster social integrations with other road users (Granié et al., 2013Granié, M. A., Pannetier, M. & Gueho, L. (2013). Developing a self-reporting method to measure pedestrian behaviors at all ages. Accident Analysis & Prevention, 50, 830-839. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.aap.2012.07.009). In this line, Deb et al. (2017bDeb, S., Strawderman, L., DuBien, J., Smith, B., Carruth, D. W. & Garrison, T. M. (2017b). Evaluating pedestrian behavior at crosswalks: Validation of a pedestrian behavior questionnaire for the US population. Accident Analysis & Prevention, 106, 191-201. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.aap.2017.05.020) claimed that positive behaviors try to reduce violations or errors, as well as to achieve the fulfillment of safety rules.
The present study aims at responding two research questions (RQ) regarding the role of VR on safe pedestrian behavior:
A 2x2 factorial experimental design with pre- and post-measures was used in this study (León & Montero, 2015León, O. G. & Montero, I. (2015). Métodos de investigación en psicología y educación: las tradiciones cuantitativa y cualitativa (4.ª). McGraw-Hill.). The two independent variables and levels were, IV1: experiencing an accident with VR (ACC)/ no experiencing an accident with VR (NOACC); and IV2: debriefing after the VR experience (DEB)/no debriefing after the VR experience (NODEB).
Sample
A total of 49 students participated in the present study. However, six participants were excluded from the analysis due to task incompletion. Thus, the sample finally comprised 43 students (67.4% women; aged between 18 and 45; M = 24.49; SD = 5.14). They attained various educational levels (master’s degree (62.8%), undergraduate degree (32.5%), and other degree (4.7%), with a predominance of the Social and Legal Sciences field of study (69.8%). They were mainly Spaniards (86%) and other nationalities such as Peruvian, Chinese, Moroccan, Venezuelan, or Colombian (14%).
Sampling was collected by convenience and snowball techniques, principally through social media networks such as WhatsApp. As Parker et al. (2019Parker, C., Scott, S. & Geddes, A. (2019). Snowball sampling. SAGE research methods foundations. https://doi.org/10.4135/9781526421036831710) noted, social networks are useful to settle initial links to develop sampling, thus, gaining a growing chain of samples. In this study, students were chosen because they fell within the targeted population, thus all of them were Spanish-speaking pedestrians involved in a student context in Madrid (Spain).
Finally, the Ethics Committee of Research of the Complutense University of Madrid (UCM) approved the project. Furthermore, an Informed Consent Statement was required, where all the main information and the ethical principles were explained and acknowledged by the participants.
Procedure
Participants were randomly assigned to one of the following four conditions: (1) ACC+DEB: participants who experienced VR as pedestrians and were involved in an accident if they transgressed the rule (e.g., cross when the light is red). The accident comprised a visual stimulus where the avatar of the participant (in first person) was hit by a car. No physical impact was deployed (see Figure 1, 2 and Supplementary material for more details). Next, they received a debriefing where they reflected upon the VR experience about actual data regarding pedestrians’ fatalities (see Supplementary material for the structure and questions of the debriefing); (2) ACC+NODEB: participants who also experienced an accident in VR as pedestrians, but without following debriefing. Instead, they had an informal conversation with researchers as emotional discharge; (3) NOACC+DEB: participants who experienced VR as pedestrians, but they were not involved in an accident. Afterwards, they received a debriefing where they reflected upon the experience in VR about actual data regarding pedestrians’ fatalities; and (4) NOACC+NODEB: Group four neither experienced an accident in VR nor received a debriefing. Data was collected over three weeks (February 7-24, 2023), with individual sessions that lasted approximately 45 minutes per person.
Figure 1. VR experience |
Figure 2. Accident condition |
Next, the sequential stages of the process are described (see Figure 3 for a flowchart).
Figure 3. Flowchart of the procedure |
Stage 1 (pre self-report measures): In this phase, all participants completed the Walking Behavior Questionnaire (see Instruments below) and a set of demographic questions. This was conducted via online using GoogleForms within 24-48 hours before the intervention started (Stage 2).
Stage 2 (pre post behavioral measures): This phase was carried out in the gym of the School of Education at Complutense University of Madrid (see Materials for further description). Participants experienced different situations, depending on the condition they were randomly assigned (ACC+DEB, ACC+NODEB, NOACC+DEB, NOACC+NODEB). Seven participants initially assigned to ACC+DEB avoided the accident during the intervention due to different reasons (e.g. they run too fast to be hit by the car). Thus, they were finally assigned to NOACC+DEB condition. Stage 2 comprised four sequential parts:
Figure 4. Familiarization scenario |
Instruments
Participants completed a pre-post questionnaire which included (a) individual and demographic information, including age, gender, nationality, or educational level. It also included a set of questions related to accidents suffered in the last two years by participants or people close to them, as well as the type of accidents and their severity; and (b) the Walking Behavior Questionnaire (WBQ) (Useche et al., 2020Useche, S. A., Alonso, F. & Montoro, L. (2020). Validation of the walking behavior questionnaire (WBQ): a tool for measuring risky and safe walking under a behavioral perspective. Journal of Transport & Health, 18. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jth.2020.100899), a 30-item self-report instrument that evaluates risky (errors and violations) and positive walking behaviors, following a Likert scale from 0 to 4 (0 = Never (at all); 1 = Almost never ; 2 = Sometimes; 3 = Frequently; 4 = Very frequently). WBQ comprises three factors: Violations (V), 16 items (α = .871; ω= .873); Errors (E), 10 items (α = .909; ω= .913); and Positive Behaviors (PB), 4 items (α = .800; ω=.814) (Useche et al., 2020Useche, S. A., Alonso, F. & Montoro, L. (2020). Validation of the walking behavior questionnaire (WBQ): a tool for measuring risky and safe walking under a behavioral perspective. Journal of Transport & Health, 18. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jth.2020.100899). The Cronbach‘s Alpha and McDonald´s Omega coefficients for the three factors ranged between good and excellent internal consistency. In the last part of the posttest, an open-ended question was raised to reflect on what effects their participation in VR had on their real life.
Materials
Location. The intervention was carried out in the gym of the School of Education-CFP, Complutense University of Madrid, an open space of 414 m2 (Figure 5) which allowed complete mobility and no risk of hitting objects or obstacles while participants were immersed in the VR environment.
Figure 5. Participant demonstration in the gym |
VR environment and equipment
The 3D environment has been implemented with Unity3D, a state-of-the-art, cross-platform game engine for VR applications. The scenario (Figure 6) has been modeled to reproduce a typical street configuration (e.g., intersections, number of lanes, crosswalks) in Madrid. To enhance the feeling of immersion and realism, it contains 3D models of urban furniture elements (e.g., buildings, decorative elements, bus shelters, traffic, and streetlights) whose textures have been generated from close-up photos of the real materials. Similarly, the virtual scene includes some dynamic elements, such as pedestrians’ avatars and circulating vehicles, to add more plausibility to the reconstructed scenario. Additionally, noise related to traffic elements (e.g., car engines and brakes) and weather conditions (e.g., rain) has been included as background, stereo sound. The virtual environment and its elements have been modeled using 3D Studio Max and texturized with Adobe Substance. Sounds have been adapted from available, online repositories.
Moreover, the functional behavior of the application has been implemented through a set of C# scripts to reproduce features such as traffic regulation, run-over simulations, the setting-up of environmental and simulation variables, the collation and storage of the experimental data in CSV (Comma Separated Values) files.
The Virtual Reality application has been built for Oculus Quest 2 headsets. The application runs on a Windows 10 Pro laptop, carrying 16 GB of RAM, an Intel i7-10750H CPU @2.60 GHz, a monitor with 144 Hz of refresh rate, and an NVIDIA GeForce RTX 2060. The headset and the laptop communicated through a dedicated, high-speed wireless connection. This configuration is particularly suitable for a smooth and comfortable VR experience.
The VR scenario is composed of two intersections regulated by two traffic lights and two pedestrian crossings. Participants started at the same point and had to reach the red cubicle to finish the simulation. They had to behave in the same way as they would if the situation were real. Those participants who were in the ACC condition could have the accident in two situations: (1) If they crossed the road diagonally at the first junction; (2) If they crossed at the second junction when the traffic light turned red (see Figure 6).
Figure 6. Different views of the VR scenario |
Data analysis
The analysis was twofold. The pedestrian behavior was tested using both self-report (i.e., using WBQ) and behavioral measures (i.e., pedestrian behavior in VR). Post data were collected in both cases. Statistical analyses were performed using both JAMOVI version 2.3.22 and SPSS version 27.
Self-report measures. Prior to the application of MANOVA, assumptions for normality and homogeneity were tested. Multivariate normality assumption was tested using Shapiro-Wilk (W = 0.737; p < .001), where the means of the variable “errors” were not normally distributed (W = 0.908; p = .002). The homogeneity was tested using Box’s M (= 33.1; p = <.0016). Excluding the errors variable, all data distributions were normal and met the requirements of MANOVA’s assumptions (W = 0.971; p = .052; = 4.29; p = .232). Then, a follow-up MANOVA explored relations between the rest of variables.
Furthermore, for descriptive analysis, mean of the pretest and the posttest were calculated and represented in graphs. MANOVA was used to determine the effect of independent variables at two levels (ACC/NOACC; DEB/NODEB) on the dependent variables (Violations, Errors, and Positive Behavior).
Behavioral measures. Pedestrian behavior in VR was recorded and analyzed. A total of 86 clips were collected (i.e., two per participant, regarding the pre-post measures). Prior to the final analysis, two different members of the research team separately evaluated 10% of the videos. Table 1 summarizes the dependent variables considered in VR.
Table 1. Dependent Variables (DVs) considered in VR | ||||||||||
|
The inter-rater reliability was almost perfect for all variables, κ = 1 (p = .005), 95% CI, except for DV2, κ = 0.6 (p = .064), 95% CI, which had a moderate agreement. Descriptive measures were used to have a first visual inspection of the data.
A generalized linear mixed model analysis (GLMM) was performed for repeated measures design with the module GAMLj in JAMOVI (Gallucci, 2019Gallucci, M. (2019). GAMLj: General analyses for linear models. [Jamovi module]. https://gamlj.github.io/). We examined the between-subject effect (i.e., ACC/NOACC; DEB/NODEB), the intra-subject effect (i.e., pre-post), and the interaction effect (group × time) as fixed effects. Participants ID was used as a cluster variable, and the participants’ intercepts were established as a random effect for addressing individual variations in the repeated measures model. Moreover, in one of the analyses, the GLMM did not converge, so McNemar’s test was used to compare the change in the distribution and determine whether the difference was not due to chance and if there was significance.
Self-report measures (WBQ)
Descriptive analysis showed a general decrease in the number of Violations (V) reported by the participants. Thus, regarding the ACC+DEB condition, Mpre=2.01; SDpre =0.554; Mpost=1.8; SDpost=0.817; the ACC+NODEB condition, Mpre=2.35; SDpre =0.409; Mpost=1.84; SDpost=0.53; the NOACC+DEB condition, Mpre=2.46; SDpre =0.526; Mpost=1.93; SDpost=0.5; and finally the NOACC+NODEB condition, Mpre=1.95; SDpre =0.769; Mpost=1.61; SDpost=0.793.
Figure 7. The mean score in the Violations factor |
Descriptive analysis revealed a general decrease too in the number of Errors (E) reported by the participants, except for the condition ACC+NODEB, where no change was found. Thus, regarding the ACC+DEB condition, Mpre = 0.91; Sdpre = 0.869; Mpost = 0.52; Sdpost = 0.377; the ACC+NODEB condition, Mpre = 1.01; SDpre = 0.314; Mpost = 1.02; Sdpost = 0.703; the NOACC+DEB condition, Mpre = 1.25; SDpre = 0.774; Mpost = 1.18; Sdpost = 0.554; and finally, the NOACC+NODEB condition, Mpre = 0.755; SDpre = 0.572; Mpost = 0.536; Sdpost = 0.499.
Figure 8. The mean score in the Errors factor |
Surprisingly, descriptive analysis revealed a general decrease too in the number of Positive Behaviors (PB) reported by the participants, also with the exception of the condition ACC+NODEB, where no change was found. Thus, regarding the ACC+DEB condition, Mpre = 2.77; Sdpre = 0.671; Mpost = 2.4; Sdpost = 0.719; the ACC+NODEB condition, Mpre = 1.83; SDpre = 0.81; Mpost = 1.83; Sdpost = 0.82; the NOACC+DEB condition, Mpre = 2.42; SDpre = 0.949; Mpost = 1.83; Sdpost = 0.82; and finally, the NOACC+NODEB condition, Mpre = 2.66; SDpre = 0.896; Mpost = 2.3; Sdpost = 0.539.
Figure 9. The mean score in the Positive Behaviour factor |
Regarding the MANOVA, the results revealed nonsignificant differences between the four groups (Pillai’s Trace = .207, F= .962; df= 9.117; p= .475). However, there was a main effect on pedestrian behavior regarding pre-post measures (Pillai’s Trace = .234, F = 7.721; df = 3.76; p < .001). In particular, univariate analysis pointed out towards a significant difference in Violations (F (1,84) = 8.60; p < .005) and marginal in Positive Behavior (F (1,84) = 3.36; p = .007). In other words, there was a significant reduction in the number of Violations (V), and marginally in the number of Positive Behaviors (PB), independently of the condition assigned. The effect size (Cohen’s f) was calculated (f = .035) by G*Power (version 3.1), which represents a small effect.
Behavioral measures in Virtual Reality (VR)
Findings regarding the measures in VR depicted mixed results. First, there was no significant difference in the number of participants who looked to the right at the first crossing (DV1) between pre-post measures. Thus, in ACC+DEB condition, fpre-post = 80%-70%; ACC+NODEB condition, fpre-post = 56%-45%; NOACC+DEB condition, fpre-post = 92%-92%; NOACC+NODEB condition, fpre-post = 73%-82%. The GLMM revealed no main effect of the variables ACC/NOACC (χ² (1) = 0.472; p = .492), DEB/NODEB (χ² (1) = 0.002; p = .964), and TIME (χ² (1) = 0.325; p = .569), but there was a significant interaction between DEB/NODEB*TIME (χ² (1) = 4.685; p = .03; pη2=.038). In other words, there were differences between pre-post, depending on whether the participants received debriefing or not, though it was small/medium effect.
Second, there was no significant difference in the number of participants who looked to the left at the second crossing (DV2) between pre-post measures. Thus, in ACC+DEB condition, fpre-post = 30%-30%; ACC+NODEB condition, fpre-post = 34%-34%; NOACC+DEB condition, fpre-post = 23%-38%; NOACC+NODEB condition, fpre-post = 26%-27%. The GLMM showed neither main effect of the variables ACC/NOACC (χ² (1) = 1.58; p = .990), DEB/NODEB (χ²(1) = 0.083; p = .773), and TIME (χ² (1) = 0.09; p = .921), nor significant interaction between them.
Third, the model did not converge when applying GLMM with DV3. This means results may be misleading or uninterpretable. Analysis of frequencies suggest there was a general increment in the number of participants who looked to the right before the bus at the second crossing (DV3) between pre-post measures. Thus, in ACC+DEB condition, fpre-post = 60%-100%; ACC+NODEB condition, fpre-post = 56%-89%; NOACC+DEB condition, fpre-post = 54%-77%; NOACC+NODEB condition, fpre-post = 64%-73%. Furthermore, the McNemar test for repeated measures suggests this difference was significant (χ² (1) = 6.00; p = .014).
Fourth, there was a significant difference in the number of participants who looked to the right after the bus at the second crossing (DV4) between pre-post measures. Thus, in ACC+DEB condition, fpre-post = 60%-80%; ACC+NODEB condition, fpre-post = 22%-44%; NOACC+DEB condition, fpre-post = 8%-31%; NOACC+NODEB condition, fpre-post = 9%-36%. The GLMM revealed a small main effect of TIME (χ² (1) = 4.546; p = .033; pη2 = .011); a marginal main effect of ACC/NOACC (χ² (1) = 3.506; p = .061), and no main effect of DEB/NODEB (χ² (1) = 0.598; p = .439). There were no significant interactions between variables.
Finally, there was a significant difference in the number of participants who crossed on red at the second crossing (DV4) between pre-post measures. Thus, in ACC+DEB condition, fpre-post = 100%-40%; ACC+NODEB condition, fpre-post = 100%-44%; NOACC+DEB condition, fpre-post = 92%-85%; NOACC+NODEB condition, fpre-post = 91%-91%. The GLMM showed a main effect of ACC/NOACC (X² (1) = 12.82; p < .001), and TIME (X² (1) = 15.57; p < .001), and no main effect of the variable DEB/NODEB (X² (1) = 1.69; p = .194). Besides, there was a significant interaction between ACC/NOACC*TIME (X² (1) = 15.04; p < .001; pη2 = .14), which represents a large effect. In other words, participants who experienced the accident in the first VR scenario were less likely to cross on red in the second one.
Figura 10. Percentage distribution of Behavioral Measures across groups |
Note. DV1= Looking right at the first crossing; DV2= Looking left at the second crossing; DV3= Looking right before the bus at the second crossing; DV4= Looking right after the bus at the second crossing; DV5= Crossing on red at the second crossing.
The present study aimed to provide evidence regarding to what extent the use of VR on road safety might improve safe pedestrian behavior. The purpose of using VR as an educational tool was to determine, on the one hand, if having an accident as a pedestrian in VR might help to improve the behavior in urban environments (RQ1); and, on the other hand, if having a reflection and debriefing on the experience in an urban VR environment might help to improve pedestrian behavior (RQ2).
Discussion regarding RQ1
Firstly, self-report measures findings (WBQ) indicate that the students significantly reduced their violations and marginally their positive behaviors after experimenting with VR. Surprisingly, in both cases, it happened regardless of the condition in which the participants were allocated. This result does not support Hypothesis 1 since having an accident in VR did not make a difference. However, it stresses the potential of VR to modify human behavior (Çakiroğlu & Gökoğlu, 2019Çakiroğlu, Ü., & Gökoğlu, S. (2019). Development of fire safety behavioral skills via virtual reality. Computers & Education, 133, 56-68. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.compedu.2019.01.014; Deb et al., 2017aDeb, S., Carruth, D. W., Sween, R., Strawderman, L. & Garrison, T. M. (2017a). Efficacy of virtual reality in pedestrian safety research. Applied Ergonomics, 65, 449-460. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.apergo.2017.03.007; Schneider et al., 2022Schneider, S., Maruhn, P., Dang, N. T., Pala, P., Cavallo, V. & Bengler, K. (2022). Pedestrian crossing decisions in virtual environments: behavioral validity in CAVEs and head-mounted displays. Human Factors, 64(7), 1210-1226. https://doi.org/10.1177/0018720820987446). One possible explanation for the counterintuitive fact that participants reduced their positive behaviors is that taking part in this study made them more aware of their pedestrian behaviors. In this sense, after going through the different stages of the process, individuals realized they were not as good pedestrians as they thought when they filled out the WBQ for the first time. As one participant stated:
(Taking part in this study) has made me realize that I am not as good a pedestrian as I thought I was, and I should be more patient with certain things… Participant nº 27, woman, 26 years old.
On the other hand, behavioral findings (VR) reveal that participants who have an accident in the VR1 environment significantly reduced the number of times they crossed on red in the VR2 and increased marginally the number of times they looked right after the bus at the second crossing. These results support Hypothesis 1 and reveal evidence of using VR to modify risky pedestrian behaviors (Luo et al., 2021Luo, H., Yang, T., Kwon, S., Li, G., Zuo, M. & Choi, I. (2021). Performing versus observing: Investigating the effectiveness of group debriefing in a VR-based safety education program. Computers & Education, 175. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.compedu.2021.104316). In particular, all participants in ACC conditions violated the norm through crossing on red in the VR1 scenario, and these data were reduced by more than half in the VR2 scenario. In contrast, all participants of the NOACC conditions continued to cross red in the VR2 scenario, except for one participant. This may explain the effect of VR in participants, modifying their behavior in the VR2 scenario (Purcell & Romjin, 2020Purcell, C. & Romijn, A. (2020). Teaching children road safety using a simulated environment. Journal of Education and Educational Development, 7(1), 44-54. https://doi.org/10.22555/joeed.v7i1.2948).
On the contrary, findings show that students barely reduced their errors after experimenting with VR. It may be due to a potential floor effect since participants were already informed of a few errors during the pre-test. Thus, there was little room for improvement during the post-test. Something similar happened regarding the number of times participants looked right at the first crossing. There was little increment during the post-test since the numbers were already high in the pre-test (above 70% in most conditions). In the case of the number of times participants turned left at the second crossing, findings reveal that there was hardly any change with very low values under all conditions. This may be strongly influenced by the direction of traffic (Osorio-García et al., 2023Osorio-García, D., Hernández-Pulgarín, G. & Escobar, D. A. (2023). Profiles of pedestrian risk behavior while crossing the street. Safety Science, 163. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ssci.2023.106120), as cars were coming only from the right. Moreover, the further analysis seems to indicate pre-post differences in the number of times participants looked right before the bus at the second crossing. Future research should test these hypotheses.
Generally, the results are promising because all participants improved. Specifically, results based on pedestrian behavior have affected crossing on red since this variable is considered to have an immediate consequence (having the accident or not). Specifically, the typical risky behaviors among participants were crossing without checking vehicular traffic or crossing at a red pedestrian light (Osorio-García et al., 2023Osorio-García, D., Hernández-Pulgarín, G. & Escobar, D. A. (2023). Profiles of pedestrian risk behavior while crossing the street. Safety Science, 163. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ssci.2023.106120). The lack of negative consequences could lead to a low-risk perception. Future research should also test this hypothesis.
Discussion regarding RQ2
Firstly, self-report findings (WBQ) indicate that participants did not modify their behavior enough by having a debriefing experience after VR in violations, errors, and positive behaviors. In other words, data does not seem to support Hypothesis 2, as there is no indication of a significant improvement in pedestrian behavior after debriefing.
Secondly, findings regarding behavioral measures (VR) reveal that participants displayed a different pattern of behavior at the first crossing (DV1), depending on whether they received debriefing or not. As such, these results do not seem to support Hypothesis 2 either since participants who have debriefing did not improve their pedestrian behavior. Regarding the rest of behavioral measures, most participants improved in DV3 and DV4, independently of the condition. Therefore, in this case the data do not seem to support Hypothesis 2 either since having a debriefing after the experience in VR did not make a difference.
A possible explanation for these unexpected results refers to the nature of the debriefing technique used, since there are different approaches to do so (Feng et al., 2021Feng, Q., Luo, H., Li, W., Chen, Y.,& Zhang, J. (2021). The moderating effect of debriefing on learning outcomes of IVR-based instruction: an experimental research. Applied Sciences, 11(21), 10426. https://doi.org/10.3390/app112110426). According to Levett-Jones & Lapkin (2014Levett-Jones, T. & Lapkin, S. (2014). A systematic review of the effectiveness of simulation debriefing in health professional education. Nurse Education Today, 34(6), e58-e63. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.nedt.2013.09.020), “it is not possible to form strong conclusions regarding a best practice model or debriefing framework” (p.e63). Elements such as length of the debriefing, level of participation (i.e., individually vs. in groups) or the debriefing format (e.g., using multimedia resources, self-guided debriefing, etc.) might have dramatic implications for the outcomes. Besides, it is important to note these conclusions were held for debriefings conducted in health professional education. As Feng et al. (2021Feng, Q., Luo, H., Li, W., Chen, Y.,& Zhang, J. (2021). The moderating effect of debriefing on learning outcomes of IVR-based instruction: an experimental research. Applied Sciences, 11(21), 10426. https://doi.org/10.3390/app112110426) suggest, types of knowledge content and particular performance tasks associated might limit or boost the potential of debriefing for learning purposes. This is especially important in the study of pedestrian behavior given that little is known about what the optimum combination of features is to enhance debriefing in this field.
General discussion
Overall, participants stated that they had experienced changes in their pedestrian behavior after taking part in this study, regardless of the condition assigned. In some cases, they did not know whether it was because of filling out the questionnaire or experiencing the VR. As one participant clearly stated:
I would say that I have been a bit more careful these days, although I can’t say for sure whether it was because of the VR or because I took the pre-test. Participant nº 5, man, 28 years old.
It could be argued that filling out the WBQ questionnaire made participants more aware of their pedestrian behaviors. As Useche et al. (2020Useche, S. A., Alonso, F. & Montoro, L. (2020). Validation of the walking behavior questionnaire (WBQ): a tool for measuring risky and safe walking under a behavioral perspective. Journal of Transport & Health, 18. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jth.2020.100899) pointed out, the WBQ can be especially useful for designing studies and educational road safety programs focused on road risk reduction and the development of safe walking behavior. Therefore, it seems reasonable that the fact that participants took part in the study has led to a change in their pedestrian behavior, regardless of the condition in which they experienced VR.
All in all, we can conclude that using VR environments might improve pedestrian behavior, especially under particular circumstances (e.g., having an accident). Some aspects of the behavior were more affected than others. For instance, there was a dramatic reduction in the number of times participants crossed with red lights, but there was not a significant change in looking at the right at the first cross. The most plausible explanation for this refers to how the participants made the connection between their behaviors and the accident. In other words, participants likely interpreted that crossing with red lights caused them to suffer an accident, whereas looking (or not looking) at the red light did not have any consequence.
Finally, the unexpected results might be due to limitations in the study, which are discussed next, as well as future avenues for research.
Limitations and Future Research
This study is not exempt from limitations. The results should be considered with caution, as the sample size is not large enough for generalization. Besides, cultural and gender differences might account for some variance in the outcomes, though the unbalanced distribution of the sample did not allow for those variables to be controlled. Gender differences in pedestrian behavior are well known so men usually display lower risk perception and higher risk behaviors than women (Useche et al., 2021Useche, S. A., Hezaveh, A. M., Llamazares, F. J. & Cherry, C. (2021). Not gendered… but different from each other? A structural equation model for explaining risky road behaviors of female and male pedestrians. Accident Analysis & Prevention, 150. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.aap.2020.105942).
Regarding the research design, future research should include another condition where participants do not engage in VR environments but fulfill the WBQ. In doing so, we would be able to determine to what extent participants change their behaviors because of the VR experience or because of conducting the WBQ.
Although both self-report and behavioral measures were taken in this study, future research should include measures in natural environments too. The final goal of using VR for learning purposes is to improve pedestrian behavior in real contexts. In this vein, there are many unknowns yet about what exactly is transferred from learning VR to real contexts (Saadati et al., 2022Saadati, M., Razzaghi, A., Rezapour, R. & Pourebrahim, K. (2022). Interventions for safety promotion of pedestrians; A scoping review. Journal of Transport & Health, 24. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jth.2021.101277). Similarly, further research should consider later post-measures to determine how long the learning from VR experiences last. Finally, future studies should pay more attention to how the debriefing is designed and developed. In doing so, we could determine what would be the best combination of features in order to optimize the learning experience.
As technology continues to advance, it is crucial to explore innovative approaches like VR to address the persistent challenges of pedestrian safety. By harnessing the power of immersive experiences, we can pave the way for a future where pedestrian accidents and fatalities become minimized, fostering a society that prioritizes the well-being and protection of all road users. Further research may help to unlock the full potential of these technologies, while a safer and more inclusive environment for pedestrians worldwide is developed.
The authors disclosed receipt of the following financial support for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article: Project VULNEUREA Grant PID2021-122290OB-C21 funded by MCIN/ AEI / 10.13039/501100011033 / “ERDF A way of making Europe”, EU; and Project SAFEDUCA VR Grant PDC2022-133381-I00 funded by MCIN/AEI/10.13039/501100011033 and by the “European Union NextGenerationEU/PRTR”.
All authors declare that there are no competing interests.
Credit author statement
Alicia López Álvarez: Conceptualization, Methodology, Validation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Resources, Data Curation, Writing - Original Draft Preparation, Writing- Reviewing and Editing, Visualization.
Luca Piovano: Conceptualization, Methodology, Software, Resources.
Francisco Luque: Conceptualization, Methodology, Software, Resources.
Carlos De Aldama: Conceptualization, Methodology, Validation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Resources, Data Curation, Writing- Reviewing and Editing, Supervision, Project administration.
The authors acknowledge the invaluable contributions of 43 participants during the research experiment and institutional stakeholders involved in the data collection. Also, the authors thank Prof. Carla Sebastián-Enesco for her suggestions during the data analysis process and Prof. Gustavo González-Cuevas for his review of the final draft of the manuscript. Besides, LLM (ChatGPT) was used during the writing process only for English curation. The authors acknowledge they are the final responsible for the ideas expressed in this manuscript.
○ | Aguilar-Reyes, C., Wozniak, D., Ham, A. & Zahabi, M. (2022). An adaptive virtual reality-based training system for pilots. In Proceedings of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society Annual Meeting (vol. 66, n.º 1, pp. 1962-1966)., CA: SAGE Publications. https://doi.org/10.1177/1071181322661063 |
○ | Assailly, J. P. (2017). Road safety education: What works? Patient education and counseling, 100, S24-S29. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pec.2015.10.017 |
○ | Baeza González, A., Usart Rodríguez, M. & Marqués Molías, L. (2023). Un análisis de las simulaciones virtuales desde la óptica del modelo TPACK. Bordón. Revista de Pedagogía, 75(4), 109-133. https://doi.org/10.13042/Bordon.2023.97585 |
○ | Çakiroğlu, Ü., & Gökoğlu, S. (2019). Development of fire safety behavioral skills via virtual reality. Computers & Education, 133, 56-68. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.compedu.2019.01.014 |
○ | Cook, D. A., Stanley, J. H., Brydges, R., Zendejas B, Szostek, J. H., Wang, T., Erwin, P. J. & Hatala, R. (2013). Comparative effectiveness of instructional design features in simulation-based education: Systematic review and meta-analysis. Medical Teacher, 35(1), e867-e898. https://doi.org/10.3109/0142159X.2012.714886 |
○ | Deb, S., Carruth, D. W., Sween, R., Strawderman, L. & Garrison, T. M. (2017a). Efficacy of virtual reality in pedestrian safety research. Applied Ergonomics, 65, 449-460. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.apergo.2017.03.007 |
○ | Deb, S., Strawderman, L., DuBien, J., Smith, B., Carruth, D. W. & Garrison, T. M. (2017b). Evaluating pedestrian behavior at crosswalks: Validation of a pedestrian behavior questionnaire for the US population. Accident Analysis & Prevention, 106, 191-201. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.aap.2017.05.020 |
○ | Delgado, A. (2021). Campañas: ¿la clave? Repetir. Revista DGT. https://revista.dgt.es/es/reportajes/2021/09SEPTIEMBRE/0923-Campanas-publicidad.shtml |
○ | Feng, Q., Luo, H., Li, W., Chen, Y.,& Zhang, J. (2021). The moderating effect of debriefing on learning outcomes of IVR-based instruction: an experimental research. Applied Sciences, 11(21), 10426. https://doi.org/10.3390/app112110426 |
○ | Ferguson, C., Van den Broek, E. L. & Van Oostendorp, H. (2020). On the role of interaction mode and story structure in virtual reality serious games. Computers & Education, 143, 103671. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.compedu.2019.103671 |
○ | Field, A. (2018). Discovering statistics using IBM SPSS statistics (5th ed.) Sage publications Ltd. |
○ | Gallucci, M. (2019). GAMLj: General analyses for linear models. [Jamovi module]. https://gamlj.github.io/ |
○ | Gardner, R. (2013). Introduction to debriefing. In Seminars in perinatology, vol. 37, n.o 3, 166-174. WB Saunders. https://doi.org/10.1053/j.semperi.2013.02.008 |
○ | Gicquel, L., Ordonneau, P., Blot, E., Toillon, C., Ingrand, P. & Romo, L. (2017). Description of various factors contributing to traffic accidents in youth and measures proposed to alleviate recurrence. Frontiers in psychiatry, 8, 94. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2017.00094 |
○ | Granié, M. A., Pannetier, M. & Gueho, L. (2013). Developing a self-reporting method to measure pedestrian behaviors at all ages. Accident Analysis & Prevention, 50, 830-839. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.aap.2012.07.009 |
○ | Hou, M., Chen, S. & Cheng, J. (2022). The effect of risk perception and other psychological factors on mobile phone use while crossing the street among pedestrians. Accident Analysis & Prevention, 170. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.aap.2022.106643 |
○ | IBM Corp. Released 2020. IBM SPSS Statistics for Mac, Version 27.0. Armonk, NY: IBM Corp. |
○ | Lee, J., Lee, H., Kim, S., Choi, M., Ko, I. S., Bae, J. & Kim, S. H. (2020). Debriefing methods and learning outcomes in simulation nursing education: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Nurse Education Today, 87. https://doi.org./10.1016/j.nedt.2020.104345 |
○ | León, O. G. & Montero, I. (2015). Métodos de investigación en psicología y educación: las tradiciones cuantitativa y cualitativa (4.ª). McGraw-Hill. |
○ | Levett-Jones, T. & Lapkin, S. (2014). A systematic review of the effectiveness of simulation debriefing in health professional education. Nurse Education Today, 34(6), e58-e63. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.nedt.2013.09.020 |
○ | Luo, H., Yang, T., Kwon, S., Li, G., Zuo, M. & Choi, I. (2021). Performing versus observing: Investigating the effectiveness of group debriefing in a VR-based safety education program. Computers & Education, 175. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.compedu.2021.104316 |
○ | Marrero Galván, J. J. & Hernández Padrón, M. (2022). La trascendencia de la realidad virtual en la educación STEM: una revisión sistemática desde el punto de vista de la experimentación en el aula. Bordón. Revista de Pedagogía, 74(4), 45-63. https://doi.org/10.13042/Bordon.2022.94179 |
○ | National Road Safety Observatory (2021). Las primeras cifras de la siniestralidad de los peatones. Dirección General de Tráfico de Madrid, España. |
○ | National Road Safety Observatory (2023). Siniestralidad mortal en vías interurbanas 2022. Dirección General de Tráfico de Madrid, España. |
○ | O’Hern, S., Stephens, A. N., Young, K. & Koppel, S., (2019). Personality traits as predictors of cyclist behavior. In: Paper Presented at the International Cycling Safety Conference – ICSC2019, 18-20 November 2019 (extended Version). Brisbane, Australia. QUT, Brisbane. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.aap.2020.105704 |
○ | Osorio-García, D., Hernández-Pulgarín, G. & Escobar, D. A. (2023). Profiles of pedestrian risk behavior while crossing the street. Safety Science, 163. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ssci.2023.106120 |
○ | Palacios Ortega, A., Pascual López, V. & Moreno Mediavilla, D. (2022). El papel de las nuevas tecnologías en la educación STEM. Bordón. Revista de Pedagogía, 74(4), 11-21. https://doi.org/10.13042/Bordon.2022.96550 |
○ | Parker, C., Scott, S. & Geddes, A. (2019). Snowball sampling. SAGE research methods foundations. https://doi.org/10.4135/9781526421036831710 |
○ | Purcell, C. & Romijn, A. (2020). Teaching children road safety using a simulated environment. Journal of Education and Educational Development, 7(1), 44-54. https://doi.org/10.22555/joeed.v7i1.2948 |
○ | Reason, J., Manstead, A., Stradling, S., Baxter, J. & Campbell, K. (1990). Errors and violations on the roads: a real distinction? Ergonomics, 33(10-11), 1315-1332. https://doi.org/10.1080/00140139008925335 |
○ | Saadati, M., Razzaghi, A., Rezapour, R. & Pourebrahim, K. (2022). Interventions for safety promotion of pedestrians; A scoping review. Journal of Transport & Health, 24. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jth.2021.101277 |
○ | Schneider, S., Maruhn, P., Dang, N. T., Pala, P., Cavallo, V. & Bengler, K. (2022). Pedestrian crossing decisions in virtual environments: behavioral validity in CAVEs and head-mounted displays. Human Factors, 64(7), 1210-1226. https://doi.org/10.1177/0018720820987446 |
○ | Secretary-General, U. N. (2020). Improving global road safety: note/by the Secretary-General. |
○ | Seo, H. J., Park, G. M., Son, M. & Hong, A. J. (2021). Establishment of virtual-reality-based safety education and training system for safety engagement. Education Sciences, 11(12), 786. https://doi.org/10.3390/educsci11120786 |
○ | Twisk, D. A., Vlakveld, W. P., Commandeur, J. J., Shope, J. T. & Kok, G. (2014). Five road safety education programmes for young adolescent pedestrians and cyclists: A multi-programme evaluation in a field setting. Accident Analysis & Prevention, 66, 55-61. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.aap.2014.01.002 |
○ | Useche, S. A., Alonso, F. & Montoro, L. (2020). Validation of the walking behavior questionnaire (WBQ): a tool for measuring risky and safe walking under a behavioral perspective. Journal of Transport & Health, 18. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jth.2020.100899 |
○ | Useche, S. A., Hezaveh, A. M., Llamazares, F. J. & Cherry, C. (2021). Not gendered… but different from each other? A structural equation model for explaining risky road behaviors of female and male pedestrians. Accident Analysis & Prevention, 150. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.aap.2020.105942 |
○ | Vankov, D. & Jankovszky, D. (2021). Effects of using headset-delivered virtual reality in road safety research: A systematic review of empirical studies. Virtual Reality & Intelligent Hardware, 3(5), 351-368. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.vrih.2021.05.005 |
○ | Wang, T., Wu, J., Zheng, P. & McDonald, M. (2010). Study of pedestrians’ gap acceptance behavior when they jaywalk outside crossing facilities. In 13th International IEEE Conference on Intelligent Transportation Systems (pp. 1295-1300). IEEE. https://doi.org/ 10.1109/ITSC.2010.5625157. |
○ | World Health Organization (2018). Global status report on road safety. |
○ | Zhou, R. & Horrey, W. J. (2010). Predicting adolescent pedestrians’ behavioral intentions to follow the masses in risky crossing situations. Transportation research part F: traffic psychology and behavior, 13(3), 153-163. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.trf.2009.12.001 |
Walking Behavior Questionnaire (WBQ) | ||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
|
1.Instruments for Data Collection
SEC I: Demographics
SEC II: Interview script for joint reflection (Debriefing)
A) For the YES accident condition:
B) For the NO accident condition:
The data set and an example of a simulation are openly available in OSF:
https://osf.io/yxenq/?view_only=48d2cd0fbde0467e88046bb961662fd4
Mejorar el comportamiento de los peatones mediante la realidad virtual: un estudio empírico
INTRODUCCIÓN. En los últimos años ha aumentado el número de peatones fallecidos en las vías urbanas, debido en gran medida a infracciones asociadas a sus comportamientos (por ejemplo, cruzar cuando el semáforo está en rojo). Se argumenta que estos comportamientos reflejan una falta de percepción del riesgo. Los programas de seguridad vial han tratado de concienciar a través de diversos métodos, utilizando muy a menudo experiencias emocionales impactantes (por ejemplo, testimonios de personas que han sufrido ellas mismas un accidente). Recientemente, la realidad virtual (RV) se ha desplegado con el objetivo de aumentar la eficacia de estos programas de seguridad. Estudios anteriores han demostrado el potencial de la RV para mejorar la seguridad de los peatones, especialmente cuando va acompañada de un debriefing y una reflexión crítica. MÉTODO. Un total de 43 participantes (M = 24.5 años; SD = 5.14; 65,12% mujeres) participaron en un estudio experimental con un diseño factorial 2x2 y medidas pre-post. Se los asignó aleatoriamente a uno de cuatro grupos (Experimentar un accidente en RV/Experimentar RV sin accidente; Tener un debriefing tras la experiencia de RV/No tener un debriefing tras la experiencia de RV). Las medidas pre-post fueron de dos tipos, (a) medidas de autoinforme y (b) medidas de comportamiento en RV. Para analizar los datos se utilizaron análisis multivariantes de la varianza (MANOVA) y modelos lineales generales mixtos (GLMM). RESULTADOS Y DISCUSIÓN. Los principales resultados revelaron que (a) los participantes informaron de una reducción general en el número de infracciones de las normas, independientemente de la condición, y (b) hubo una reducción significativa en el número de infracciones cometidas en RV (es decir, cruzar cuando el semáforo está en rojo) en la condición en la que los participantes habían experimentado previamente un accidente. Estos resultados respaldan el potencial del uso de entornos de RV para mejorar el comportamiento relacionado con la seguridad de los peatones. Se delinean las implicaciones para futuras investigaciones.
Palabras clave: Realidad virtual (RV), Técnicas de debriefing, Programas de seguridad, Comportamiento de los peatones, Riesgo de accidentes, Seguridad.
Améliorer le comportement des piétons par la réalité virtuelle : une étude empirique
INTRODUCTION. Au cours des dernières années, le nombre de piétons décédés sur les voies urbaines a augmenté, principalement en raison d’infractions associées à leurs comportements (par exemple, traverser lorsque le feu est rouge). On soutient que ces comportements reflètent un manque de perception du risque. Les programmes de sécurité routière ont tenté de sensibiliser par divers moyens, utilisant souvent des expériences émotionnelles frappantes (par exemple, des témoignages de personnes ayant elles-mêmes été victimes d’un accident). Récemment, la Réalité Virtuelle (RV) a été déployée dans le but d’accroître l’efficacité de ces programmes de sécurité. Des études antérieures ont démontré le potentiel de la RV pour améliorer la sécurité des piétons, notamment lorsqu’elle est accompagnée d’un débriefing et d’une réflexion critique. MÉTHODE. Un total de 43 participants (M = 24,5 ans ; SD = 5,14 ; 65,12 % femmes) ont pris part à une étude expérimentale avec un plan factoriel 2x2 et des mesures pré-post. Ils ont été assignés de manière aléatoire à l’un des quatre groupes (Expérience d’un accident en RV / Expérience de RV sans accident ; avoir un débriefing après l’expérience en RV / ne pas avoir de débriefing après l’expérience en RV). Les mesures pré-post étaient de deux types, (a) des mesures d’auto-déclaration et (b) des mesures de comportement en RV. Pour analyser les données, des analyses de variance multivariées (MANOVA) et des modèles linéaires généraux mixtes (GLMM) ont été utilisés. RÉSULTATS ET DISCUSSION. Les principaux résultats ont révélé que (a) les participants ont signalé une réduction générale du nombre d’infractions aux règles, indépendamment de la condition, et (b) il y a eu une réduction significative du nombre d’infractions commises en RV (c’est-à-dire traverser lorsque le feu est rouge) dans la condition où les participants avaient précédemment vécu un accident. Ces résultats soutiennent le potentiel de l’utilisation des environnements de RV pour améliorer le comportement lié à la sécurité des piétons. Les implications pour de futures recherches sont énoncées.
Mots-clés : Réalité virtuelle (RV), Techniques de débriefing, Programmes de sécurité, Comportement des piétons, Risque d’accidents, Sécurité.
Alicia López-Álvarez (corresponding author)
Master’s degree student in Educational Research master from the Complutense University of Madrid. She graduated in Early Childhood Education and Primary Education with a major in English. Her research has focused on the use of VR as an educational tool and its relation to road safety and pedestrian behavior.
ORCID ID: https://orcid.org/0009-0002-3208-6112
E-mail: aliclo14@ucm.es
Correspondence address: Department of Research and Psychology in Education, Complutense University of Madrid, Madrid 28040 (Spain).
Luca Piovano
PhD in Computer Science by the University of Turin (Italy). He is currently the scientifical director of the Virtual Reality and Data Visualization Department at the Centre for Energy Efficiency (CEDINT) of Polytechnic University of Madrid, where he has been working since 2012. He has participated in several R&D projects, funded at both national and European level, applying Virtual/Augmented Reality and Visual Analysis techniques, among the others, to: develop digital twins of buildings for monitoring their energy consumption; support new therapeutic strategies for the treatment of alcohol use disorders; simulate possible collisions between Vulnerable Road Users and vehicles in urban traffic scenarios; and prepare educational tools to learn about raw materials and their widespread usage in current society.
ORCID ID: https://orcid.org/0000-0001-9882-0725
E-mail: luca.piovano@upm.es
Francisco Luque
Telecommunications engineer with a master’s degree in Software and Communications Systems. He has worked as a technical researcher in the Virtual Reality and Data Visualization department in the Centre for Energy Efficiency (CEDINT-UPM) since his graduation in 2008. In this period, he has participated in numerous national and European projects where, being the main technical responsible of the group, he has gained great experience concerning 3D graphic engines, virtual reality systems, 3D modeling and texturing and code programming for the implementation of immersive experiences in the area of Virtual and Augmented Reality. Other skills include analysis and visualization of spatial temporal complex data using web based tools.
ORCID ID: https://orcid.org/0000-0001-8827-9971
E-mail: fp.luque@upm.es
Carlos de Aldama
PhD in Educational Psychology at Autonomous University of Madrid. He is Assistant Professor at the department of Research and Psychology in Education, Faculty of Education, Complutense University of Madrid. His interests are mainly focused on how digital technologies are reshaping the different dimensions of human nature and society. In particular, he has largely researched how to integrate them in educational settings to enhance meaningful learning and how this is linked with higher mental processes, such as reasoning and thought.
ORCID ID: https://orcid.org/0000-0001-8832-2618
E-mail: caaldama@ucm.es